top of page

The Neuroscience of Zoom Fatigue


Ever felt mentally exhausted following a virtual interaction?

During COVID-19, many of us have been increasingly reliant on videoconferencing systems for learning, working, and socializing. While these tools are great for helping us stay connected, it has also given rise to a phenomenon known as ‘zoom fatigue’. First described by Stanford professor Jeremy Bailenson, ‘zoom fatigue’ refers to feelings of mental and physical exhaustion following virtual interactions [1].


Overall, ‘zoom fatigue’ can be attributed to an effort-reward imbalance. Virtual interactions use up more cognitive resources and require more effort, however we do not perceive them as more rewarding than in-person interactions [2]. This imbalance eventually leads to feelings of fatigue [3]. To explore this idea further, let’s zoom in to several features of virtual interactions.



The setup

You log into your first online class for the day and are greeted by the various faces of your classmates.


However, these virtual faces appear much larger and closer than in-person. Bailenson describes how we are all interacting with virtual others at distances equivalent to being 60cm away from another person [1]. This distance is classified as ‘intimate’ [4] and is usually only reserved for close friends and family members. If we aren’t emotionally close to these individuals, this closeness may indicate violations of person space. In response, the sympathetic nervous system may become activated, triggering a fight-flight-freeze response [5].


To make matters worse, faces as stimuli are super distracting, making it harder to concentrate on the task at hand. Faces reliably express various biological and social cues, and are therefore hard to ignore. There are even specialized brain regions dedicated to facial processing and recognition (e.g. the fusiform face area and the occipital face area [6]), and one study found that even neutral faces as distractors slowed down participants’ task completion [7].


All of this means that right from the outset, we start meetings feeling anxious and

easily distractible.



Figure 1. Virtual Learning [8].



The communication

As you are listening to the class, your internet connection drops out for a few seconds and you miss out on a chunk of what’s being said.


Studies have shown that video communication increases cognitive load by providing more information to be interpreted [9]. It also places a big demand on internet networks, making lags more common and requiring extra effort to fill in the gaps. There is additional effort required in perceiving and sending out nonverbal cues [1]. For example, sideway glances may not mean the same online as they do in person and we may exaggerate nods or gestures to indicate agreement. The accumulation of all these cues may wear us out.



Figure 2. Photo of a woman with computer. Fatigue can be caused by the increased cognitive load of video communication [10].



The social reward

The teacher splits your class up into breakout rooms. You start discussing the topic but the conversation feels awkward and forced, eventually stalling as you all sit silently.


How does this play out in your brain? Research has found that the neuropeptides oxytocin and vasopressin (involved in attachment and social bonding) modulate dopaminergic pathways, meaning that we find social interactions rewarding in general [11]. However the format of social interactions also plays a role - an fMRI study found that in-person interactions activate the brain’s reward processing regions (e.g. anterior cingulate cortex, ventral striatum, amygdala) more than virtual interactions [12].


This may be due to the lack of direct eye contact during virtual interactions. Under normal circumstances, shared gaze enhances social cognition and is linked to reward processing (increased ventral striatum activation) [13], allowing us to express intimacy, empathy, and likeability [14]. However during virtual interactions it is difficult to have mutual gaze as looking at the camera means sacrificing the ability to look into other people’s eyes. This means that we do not get as much reward from eye contact as we normally would during in-person interactions.


Combined with research suggesting that audio or video delays of even 1.2 seconds lead to perceptions of decreased empathy and attentiveness [15], it is no wonder that we find virtual interactions less rewarding than live interactions.



Figure 3. Working Home. Research has found that audio or video delays and lack of eye contact is associated with decreased social cognition, empathy and attentiveness. This means individuals may find video interactions less rewarding than live ones [16].



The ease of multi-tasking

Bored from the class and lack of meaningful social connection, you end up becoming distracted. You half-heartedly pay attention while texting your friend, checking an unimportant email, and helping your sister with her homework.


All of this multitasking creates a sense of novelty. This in turn activates our dopaminergic system, meaning that we are effectively rewarded for losing focus [17]. Our prefrontal cortex also has a novelty bias, making it more likely for us to seek out novelty in the face of boredom [18].


However, constant multitasking comes at a cost. There are increased neurocognitive demands in the frontoparietal control network and dorsal attention network [19] which are regions associated with task switching. One study found that heavy multitaskers had lower brain density in the anterior cingulate cortex - an area responsible for impulse control, decision-making, and empathy [20]. This can lead to decreased productivity and feelings of fatigue.



Figure 4. Computer Work. Technology makes it easier to multi-task, which can result in feelings of fatigue [21].



However, it’s not all doom and gloom. For all the zoom fatigue we’ve experienced, we’ve also been able to have virtual birthdays, catch-ups, weddings, and funerals. Many organizations are also catching on to just how fatiguing virtual meetings can be, and have started designing platforms that replicate a more natural experience. This includes platforms such as gather.town, Mozilla Hubs, and Nottopia, which all incorporate avatars and elements of physical space. On an individual level, the first step to combating zoom fatigue may simply involve being more mindful of all the different ways in which virtual communication systems can place a strain on us.



References:

[1] Bailenson JN. Nonverbal overload: A theoretical argument for the causes of Zoom fatigue. Technology, Mind, and Behavior. 2021 Feb 23;2(1).


[2] Lee J. A neuropsychological exploration of Zoom fatigue. Psychiatric Times. 2020 Nov 17;37(11):38-9.


[3] Wylie GR, Genova HM, DeLuca J, Dobryakova E. The relationship between outcome prediction and cognitive fatigue: a convergence of paradigms. Cognitive, Affective, & Behavioral Neuroscience. 2017 Aug;17(4):838-49.


[4] Brown N. Edward T. Hall: Proxemic Theory, 1966. Center for Spatially Integrated Social Science. University of California, Santa Barbara. http://www. csiss. org/classics/content/13 Read. 2001;18:2007.


[5] Kennedy DP, Gläscher J, Tyszka JM, Adolphs R. Personal space regulation by the human amygdala. Nature neuroscience. 2009 Oct;12(10):1226-7.


[6] Kanwisher N, Yovel G. The fusiform face area: a cortical region specialized for the perception of faces. Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society B: Biological Sciences. 2006 Dec 29;361(1476):2109-28.


[7] Parks EL, Kim SY, Hopfinger JB. The persistence of distraction: A study of attentional biases by fear, faces, and context. Psychonomic bulletin & review. 2014 Dec;21(6):1501-8.


[8] Hassan, M. [Internet] Virtual Learning. 2014. Available from: https://pxhere.com/en/photo/1626640


[9] Hinds PJ. The cognitive and interpersonal costs of video. Media Psychology. 1999 Dec 1;1(4):283-311.


[10] Q000024. [Internet] Photo of woman with computer. 2021. Available at: https://pxhere.com/en/photo/1636774


[11] Donaldson ZR, Young LJ. Oxytocin, vasopressin, and the neurogenetics of sociality. Science. 2008 Nov 7;322(5903):900-4.


[12] Redcay E, Dodell-Feder D, Pearrow MJ, Mavros PL, Kleiner M, Gabrieli JD, Saxe R. Live face-to-face interaction during fMRI: a new tool for social cognitive neuroscience. Neuroimage. 2010 May 1;50(4):1639-47.


[13] Schilbach L, Wilms M, Eickhoff SB, Romanzetti S, Tepest R, Bente G, Shah NJ, Fink GR, Vogeley K. Minds made for sharing: initiating joint attention recruits reward-related neurocircuitry. Journal of cognitive neuroscience. 2010 Dec 1;22(12):2702-15.


[14] Senju A, Johnson MH. The eye contact effect: mechanisms and development. Trends in cognitive sciences. 2009 Mar 1;13(3):127-34.


[15] Schoenenberg K, Raake A, Koeppe J. Why are you so slow?–Misattribution of transmission delay to attributes of the conversation partner at the far-end. International journal of human-computer studies. 2014 May 1;72(5):477-87


[16] Moloney. M [Internet] Working Home. n.d. Available from: https://stocksnap.io/photo/working-home-O8EAEFRYBP .


[17] Doidge N. The brain that changes itself. Toronto: Penguin Books; 2007.


[18] Matsumoto M, Matsumoto K, Tanaka K. Effects of novelty on activity of lateral and medial prefrontal neurons. Neuroscience research. 2007 Feb 1;57(2):268-76.


[19] Madore KP, Wagner AD. Multicosts of Multitasking. InCerebrum: the Dana forum on brain science 2019 Mar (Vol. 2019). Dana Foundation.


[20] Loh KK, Kanai R. Higher media multi-tasking activity is associated with smaller gray-matter density in the anterior cingulate cortex. Plos one. 2014 Sep 24;9(9):e106698.


[21] [Internet] Computer Work. 2017. Available from: https://pxhere.com/en/photo/710514

40 views0 comments

Recent Posts

See All
bottom of page